Boys tend to outperform girls in mathematics. This is particularly the case among students who achieve the highest grades
This can be significant as gender differences in math performance can dictate how young women choose educational pathways and careers. Despite more women graduating college, women remain under-represented in the higher-paying and math-intensive STEM fields (science, technology, engineering and math). They also tend to choose degrees with about 6% lower average earnings than men and 10% lower among the highest earners, those above the 90th percentile of earnings.
How can we address this gender gap in math education?
How differently do girls and boys perform in mathematics?
At the age of 11, boys are four percent more likely to achieve the expected standard in math than girls. They are also eight percentage points more likely to achieve the higher standard in standardized tests.
This pattern is seen not only in the United States but across the world in both developed and under-developed countries.
Recent evidence from several countries shows that while the gender gap in math is almost negligible in primary school, it more than triples between the ages of 9-10 and 15-16 This gap in math performance equates to girls missing around four months of schooling (Woessmann, 2016).
Girls’ underperformance in math represents a potential loss of talent, which may lead to lower productivity and economic growth (Hsieh et al, 2019).
There is no biological reason why boys should be better at math than girls. In fact, if one were to choose two people at random, the differences across a multitude of traits and abilities between a man and a woman (or a boy and a girl) will be much smaller than the differences between two women/girls or between two men/boys (Hyde, 2005; Bertrand, 2020).
Even for the small number of traits for which sex differences are significant (such as spatial thinking, sensation-seeking, physical aggression and some sexual behaviors), it is increasingly becoming more difficult to justify these differences as biologically driven, given that the newest findings in neuroscience suggest that the brain changes and adapts according to the social environment (Rippon, 2019).
What role do gender stereotypes play?
Gender stereotypes are fixed ideas about what someone of a particular gender is like (descriptive) or should be like (prescriptive), simply for the mere fact of belonging to a certain gender group.
Common gender stereotypes regarding mathematics are that boys are better than girls and that girls should not pursue hard-core STEM fields. These stereotypes are often held by both student’s parents and teachers.
Stereotypes influence women’s and girls’ preferences and expectations. Prescriptive stereotypes such as conservative beliefs about the role of women in society, rather than biology, have long been named as a factor that can explain much of the gender gap in mathematical attainment between boys and girls across countries (Guiso et al, 2008; Nollenberger et al, 2016).
Social expectations lower women’s incentives to do well. The lower performance of girls versus boys in math may also be the result of girls internalizing a socially constructed behavior in line with widely held descriptive gender stereotypes (for example, that girls shy away from competition and take lower risks) in contexts in which the task at hand has a strong gender stereotype associated to it, such as in mathematical assessments (Niederle, 2017; Iriberri and Rey-Biel, 2019).
Stereotypes also constrain women’s and girls’ choices, even when they do not internalize them. One study finds that teachers with more implicit gender stereotypical beliefs about girls’ ability to do math (as measured by the Gender-Science Implicit Association Test) advise girls to pursue less math-intensive subjects (Carlana, 2019). It also finds that girls taught by these teachers display lower confidence in the subject.
The influence of social psychology has led to a growing acceptance among economists that stereotyping behavior may also arise from a fast and unconscious (or ‘implicit’) thought process as much as from a slow and conscious process (Bertrand et al, 2005; Kahneman, 2011).
Implicit stereotyping behavior occurs even under scenarios of ‘perfect information’. Whereas economists have traditionally assumed that stereotypes kick in when we know little about a person, new evidence is beginning to emerge about stereotyping occurring even in family and classroom settings where parents and teachers know a lot about their children and students (Alesina et al, 2018; Dossi et al, 2021a, 2021b).
Either because girls receive less resources from parents and teachers when they pursue male-dominated subjects such as math, or because girls internalize these gender-stereotypical preferences, the end result is that girls end up choosing subjects of study that are ‘believed appropriate’ for their gender, and/or do worse in male-dominated subjects such as math.
How is policy being reshaped?
When unconscious gender stereotyping is at play, ‘coercive policies’ that explicitly tell people what to do may not be as effective as interventions involving small nudges (Bohnet et al, 2016). A major objective of behavioral policy interventions is to avoid unconscious stereotypes entering the decision-making process in the first place.
One type of intervention consists of tackling implicit stereotyping on the part of teachers (and parents) by using awareness techniques so that more conscious (unbiased) processes kick in. A popular tool for addressing unconscious stereotyping is revealing biases. This can be done, for example, through an implicit association test (IAT), a computer-based tool developed by social psychologists, which is designed to minimize the risk of social desirability bias (Greenwald et al, 2009).
An increasing number of firms and institutions, including Harvard University, administer the IAT to their employees. In an educational context, one study shows that revealing stereotypes to teachers via the IAT could be a powerful intervention to decrease gender discrimination in grading (Alesina et al, 2018). Other awareness interventions targeting decision-makers are ‘unconscious bias training’ and ‘perspective-taking training’.
An alternative to awareness interventions is the implementation of measures that change the context in which decisions are made, so that a more conscious process comes into effect. Researchers suggest that a large part of the gender gap in math at high levels can be explained by the different ways in which men and women respond to competitive test-taking environments (Niederle and Vesterlund, 2010).
The rise in girls’ underperformance in math has been attributed to a higher aversion to competitive pressure (Iriberri and Rey-Biel, 2019). Others show that girls outperform boys in all tests but to a greater extent when the stakes are low (Azmat et al, 2016). These results can help policy-makers to design tests that reduce gender differences in performance resulting from factors unrelated to true ability.